Skip to main content
Log in

The bored mind is a guiding mind: toward a regulatory theory of boredom

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

By presenting and synthesizing findings on the character of boredom, the article advances a theoretical account of the function of the state of boredom. The article argues that the state of boredom should be understood as a functional emotion that is both informative and regulatory of one's behavior. Boredom informs one of the presence of an unsatisfactory situation and, at the same time, it motivates one to pursue a new goal when the current goal ceases to be satisfactory, attractive or meaningful. Boredom ultimately promotes both movement and the restoration of the perception that one's activities are meaningful and congruent with one's overall projects.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. http://www.pewinternet.org/2015/04/01/us-smartphone-use-in-2015/pi_2015-04-01_smartphones_25/

References

  • Acee, T. W., Kim, H., Kim, H. J., Kim, J., Hsiang-Ning, R. C., Kim, M., Cho, Y., & Wicker, F. W. (2010). Academic boredom in under-and over-challenging situations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35(1), 17–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ahmed, S. M. S. (1990). Psychometric properties of the boredom proneness scale. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 71, 963–966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. W. (1961). Pattern and growth in personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, N. H. (1965). Averaging versus adding as a stimulus-combination rule in impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. S. (2007). Pleasant, pleasurable, and positive activities. In L. L’Abate (Ed.), Low-cost approaches to promote physical and mental health. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Angrilli, A., Cherubini, P., Pavese, A., & Manfredini, S. (1997). The influence of affective factors on time perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 972–982.

    Google Scholar 

  • Avni-Babad, D., & Ritov, I. (2003). Routine and the perception of time. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 132, 543–555.

    Google Scholar 

  • Banse, R., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(3), 614–636.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barbalet, J. M. (1999). Boredom and social meaning. The British Journal of Sociology, 50(4), 631–646.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barmack, J. E. (1937). Boredom and other factors in the physiology of mental effort: An exploratory study. Archives of Psychology, 31, 9–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barwick, F., Arnett, P., & Slobounov, S. (2012). EEG correlates of fatigue during administration of a neuropsychological test battery. Clinical Neurophysiology, 123(2), 278–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1252–1265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belton, T., & Priyadharshini, E. (2007). Boredom and schooling: A cross-disciplinary exploration. Cambridge Journal of Education, 37(4), 579–595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bench, S. W., & Lench, H. C. (2013). On the function of boredom. Behavioral Sciences, 3(3), 459–472.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1967). Arousal and reinforcement. In A. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation. University of Nebraska Press: Lincoln.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, H. E. (1975). Boredom and the ready-made life. Social Research, 42, 512–537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binnema, D. (2004). Interrelations of psychiatric patient experiences of boredom and mental health. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 25, 833–842.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blaszczynski, A., McConaghy, N., & Frankova, A. (1990). Boredom proneness in pathological gambling. Psychological Reports, 67(1), 35–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Block, R. A., & Reed, M. A. (1978). Remembered duration: Evidence for a contextual-change hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4(6), 656–665.

    Google Scholar 

  • Block, R. A., & Zakay, D. (1997). Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4(2), 184–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braby, C. D., Harris, D., & Muir, H. C. (1993). A psychophysiological approach to the assessment of work underload. Ergonomics, 36(9), 1035–1042.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bull, P. E. (1987). Posture and Gesture (International series in experimental social psychology, volume 16). Oxford: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chabot, R. J., & Serfontein, G. (1996). Quantitative electroencephalographic profiles of children with attention deficit disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 40(10), 951–963.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chanel, G., Rebetez, C., Bétrancourt, M., & Pun, T. (2008, October). Boredom, engagement and anxiety as indicators for adaptation to difficulty in games. Proceedings of MindTrek ‘08, pp. 13-17. Tampere, Finland. doi:10.1145/1457199.1457203.

  • Chaplin, W. F., John, O. P., & Goldberg, L. R. (1988). Conceptions of states and traits: Dimensional attributes with ideals as prototypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(4), 541–557.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charlton, J., & Hertz, R. (1989). Guarding against boredom. Security specialists in the US Air Force. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 18, 299–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chin, A., Markey, A., Bhargava, S., Kassam, K. S., & Loewenstein, G. (2017). Bored in the USA: Experience sampling and boredom in everyday life. Emotion, 17(2), 359–368. doi:10.1037/emo0000232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. M. (1984). Properties of the internal clock. In J. Gibbon & L. Allan (Eds.), Timing and time perception (Vol. 423, pp. 566–582). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., McCarthy, R., & Selikowitz, M. (1998). EEG analysis in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A comparative study of two subtypes. Psychiatry Research, 81(1), 19–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., & Conway, M. (1994). Affective causes and consequences of social information processing. In R. Wyer & T. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd ed., pp. 3 2 3–417). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

  • Cohn, M. A., Fredrickson, B. L., Brown, S. L., Mikels, J. A., & Conway, A. M. (2009). Happiness unpacked: Positive emotions increase life satisfaction by building resilience. Emotion, 9(3), 361.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cordes, S., Williams, C. L., & Meck, W. H. (2007). Common representations of abstract quantities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(3), 156–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P. T, McCrae, R. R., & Arenberg, D. (1980). Enduring dispositions in adult males. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 793–800.

  • Craig, S. D., D'Mello, S., Witherspoon, A., & Graesser, A. (2008). Emote aloud during learning with AutoTutor: Applying the facial action coding system to cognitive–affective states during learning. Cognition and Emotion, 22(5), 777–788.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper Perennial.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1993). The evolving self: A psychology for the third millennium. New York: Harper Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (1988). Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, M. L., Gao, F., & Thornburg, K. M. (2016). Boredom in the workplace a new look at an old problem. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 58(2), 279–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahlen, E. R., Martin, R. C., Ragan, K., & Kuhlman, M. M. (2005). Driving anger, sensation seeking, impulsiveness, and boredom proneness in the prediction of unsafe driving. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 37(2), 341–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damrad-Frye, R., & Laird, J. D. (1989). The experience of boredom: The role of the self-perception of attention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 315–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danckert, J. (2013). Descent of the doldrums. Scientific American Mind, 24(3), 54–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danckert, J. A., & Allman, A. A. A. (2005). Time flies when you’re having fun: Temporal estimation and the experience of boredom. Brain and Cognition, 59(3), 236–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danckert, J., & Merrifield, C. (2016). Boredom, sustained attention and the default mode network. Experimental Brain Research, 1–12.

  • Daniels, L. M., Tze, V. M., & Goetz, T. (2015). Examining boredom: Different causes for different coping profiles. Learning and Individual Differences, 37, 255–261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daschmann, E. C., Goetz, T., & Stupnisky, R. H. (2011). Testing the predictors of boredom at school: Development and validation of the precursors to boredom scales. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(3), 421–440.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Chenne, T. K. (1988). Boredom as a clinical issue. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 25, 71–81.

  • De Chenne, T. K., & Moody, A. J. (1988). Boredom: Theory and therapy. The Psychotherapy Patient, 3(3–4), 17–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drory, A. (1982). Boredom proneness---the development and correlates of a new scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 4–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eastwood, J. D., Cavaliere, C., Fahlman, S. A., & Eastwood, A. E. (2007). A desire for desires: Boredom and its relation to alexithymia. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(6), 1035–1045.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eastwood, J. D., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., & Smilek, D. (2012). The unengaged mind defining boredom in terms of attention. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 482–495.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1984). Expression and the nature of emotion. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 319–343). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1978). Facial action coding system: Investigator’s guide. Palo Altom: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elpidorou, A. (2014). The bright side of boredom. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elpidorou, A. (2015). The quiet alarm. Aeon Magazine. https://aeon.co/essays/life-without-boredom-would-be-a-nightmare.

  • Elpidorou, A. (2016). The significance of boredom: A Sartrean reading. In. D. Dahlstrom, A. Elpidorou, & W. Hopp, Philosophy of mind and phenomenology: Conceptual and empirical approaches (pp. 268–283). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elpidorou, A. (2017). The moral dimensions of boredom: A call for research. Review of General Psychology, 21(1), 30–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elpidorou, A. (in press). The good of boredom. Philosophical Psychology.

  • Epstein, S. (1979). The stability of behavior: I. On predicting most of the people most of the time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1097–1126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield: Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fahlman, S. A., Mercer, K. B., Gaskovski, P., Eastwood, A. E., & Eastwood, J. D. (2009). Does a lack of life meaning cause boredom? Results from psychometric, longitudinal, and experimental analyses. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28(3), 307–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fahlman, S. A., Mercer-Lynn, K. B., Flora, D. B., & Eastwood, J. D. (2013). Development and validation of the multidimensional state boredom scale. Assessment, 20, 68–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fan, X., Zhou, Q., Liu, Z., & Xie, F. (2015). Electroencephalogram assessment of mental fatigue in visual search. Bio-medical Materials and Engineering, 26(s1), 1455–1463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farmer, R., & Sundberg, N. D. (1986). Boredom proneness--the development and correlates of a new scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50(1), 4–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fenichel, O. (1951). On the psychology of boredom. In D. Rapaport (Ed.), Organization and pathology of thought: Selected sources (pp. 349–361). New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, C. D. (1993). Boredom at work: A neglected concept. Human Relations, 46, 395–417.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, D. W. (1961). Effects of monotonous and restricted stimulation. In D. W. Fiske & S. R. Maddi (Eds.), Functions of varied experience (pp. 106–145). Homewood: Dorsey Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, D. W., & Maddi, S. R. (1961). Functions of varied experience. Homewood: Dorsey Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flaherty, M. G., Freidin, B., & Sautu, R. (2005). Variation in the perceived passage of time: A cross-national study. Social Psychology Quarterly, 68, 400–410.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fleeson, W., Malanos, A. B., & Achille, N. M. (2002). An intraindividual process approach to the relationship between extraversion and positive affect: is acting extraverted as "good" as being extraverted? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(6), 1409–1422.

  • Fraisse, P. (1963). The psychology of time (J. Leith, Trans. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 300–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2013). Positive emotions broaden and build. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 47(1), 53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L., & Cohn, M. A. (2008). Positive emotions. In M. Lewis, J. Haviland-Jones & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 777–796) (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fridhandler, B. M. (1986). Conceptual note on state, trait, and the state–trait distinction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(1), 169–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gabriel, M. (1988). Boredom: Exploration of a developmental perspective. Clinical Social Work Journal, 16, 156–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Game, A. M. (2007). Workplace boredom coping: Health, safety, and HR implications. Personnel Review, 36, 701–721.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gana, K., & Akremi, M. (1998). L'échelle de Disposition à l'Ennui (EDE): Adaptation française et validation du Boredom Proneness Scale (BP).L'année. Psychologique, 98(3), 429–450.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gasper, K., & Middlewood, B. L. (2014). Approaching novel thoughts: Understanding why elation and boredom promote associative thought more than distress and relaxation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 52, 50–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geiwitz, P. J. (1966). Structure of boredom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 592–600.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giakoumis, D., Vogiannou, A., Kosunen, I., Moustakas, K., Tzovaras, D., & Hassapis, G. (2010) Identifying psychophysiological correlates of boredom and negative mood induced during HCI. BIOSTEC 2010, Valencia, Spain, pp. 3–12.

  • Gibbon, J., Church, R., & Meck, W. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423, 52–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goetz, T., & Frenzel, A. C. (2006). Phänomenologie schulischer Langeweile. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 38(4), 149–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goetz, T., Pekrun, R., Hall, N., & Haag, L. (2006). Academic emotions from a social-cognitive perspective: Antecedents and domain specificity of students' affect in the context of Latin instruction. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(2), 289–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Hall, N. C., Nett, U. E., Pekrun, R., & Lipnevich, A. A. (2014). Types of boredom: An experience sampling approach. Motivation and Emotion, 38(3), 401–419.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, Y. K., Eastwood, J. D., LaGuardia, J., & Danckert, J. (2011). Boredom: An emotional experience distinct from apathy, anhedonia, or depression. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 30(6), 647.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, A., Wilkinson, R., McGown, A., & Jovanoska, A. (1997). The psychometric properties of the boredom proneness scale: An examination of its validity. Psychological Studies, 42, 85–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grassian, S. (2006). Psychiatric effects of solitary confinement. Wash. UJL & Pol'y, 22, 325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenson, R. R. (1953). On boredom. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 1, 7–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grondin, S., & Macar, F. (1992). Dividing attention between temporal and nontemporal tasks: A performance operating characteristics—POC—Analysis. In F. Macar, V. Pouthas, & W. J. Friedman (Eds.), Towards bridging the gap (pp. 119–128). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grubb, E. A. (1975). Assembly line boredom and individual differences in recreation participation. Journal of Leisure Research, 7(4), 256–269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, J. A. (1981). Attention, personality, and the self-regulation of mood: Absorbing interest and boredom. Progress in Experimental Personality Research, 10, 281315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, J. A., Haier, R. J., & Buchsbaum, M. S. (1984). Intrinsic enjoyment and boredom coping scales: Validation with personality, evoked potential, and attention measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 5, 183–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, J., & Trope, Y. (2013). When time flies: How abstract and concrete mental construal affect the perception of time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 336–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M. B. (2000). Correlates and characteristics of boredom proneness and boredom. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 576–598.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartocollis, P. (1972). Time as a dimension of affects. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 20, 92–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasselton, M. G., & Ketelaar, T. (2006). Irrational emotions or emotional wisdom? The evolutionary psychology of emotions and behavior. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Affect in social thinking and behavior (pp. 21–39). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Havermans, R. C., Vancleef, L., Kalamatianos, A., & Nederkoorn, C. (2015). Eating and inflicting pain out of boredom. Appetite, 85, 52–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1955). Drives and the CNS (conceptual nervous system). Psychological Review, 62, 243–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heidegger, M. (1983). Die Grundbegriffe der Metaphysik. Welt-Endlichkeit-Einsamkeit. Frankfurt am Main: Vittorio Klostermann, (The Fundamental Concepts of Metaphysics: World, finitude, solitude. Indiana University Press, 2001).

  • Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model of interest development. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 111–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, A. B., & Perkins, R. E. (1985). Towards a model of boredom. British Journal of Psychology, 76(2), 235-240.

  • Hitchcock, E. M., Dember, W. N., Warm, J. S., Moroney, B. W., & See, J. E. (1999). Effects of cueing and knowledge of results on workload and boredom in sustained attention. Human Factors, 41, 365–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunter, A., & Eastwood, J. D. (2016). Does state boredom cause failures of attention? Examining the relations between trait boredom, state boredom, and sustained attention. Experimental Brain Research. doi:10.1007/s00221-016-4749-7.

  • Hunter, J. A., Abraham, E. H., Hunter, A. G., Goldberg, L. C., & Eastwood, J. D. (2016). Personality and boredom proneness in the prediction of creativity and curiosity. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 48–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M. (1984). Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 3, pp. 179-236), Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

  • Iso-Ahola, S. E., & Weissinger, E. (1987). Leisure and boredom. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 5, 356–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jiang, Y., Lianekhammy, J., Lawson, A., Guo, C., Lynam, D., Joseph, J. E., et al. (2009). Brain responses to repeated visual experience among low and high sensation seekers: Role of boredom susceptibility. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 173(2), 100–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone, T., & Scherer, K. R. (2000). Vocal communication of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 220–235). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanevsky, L., & Keighley, T. (2003). To produce or not to produce? Understanding boredom and the honor in underachievement. Roeper Review: A Journal on Gifted Education, 26, 20–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmarkar, U. R., & Buonomano, D. V. (2007). Timing in the absence of clocks: Encoding time in neural network states. Neuron, 53, 427–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kass, S. J., Beede, K. E., & Vodanovich, S. J. (2010). Self-report measures of distractibility as correlates of simulated driving performance. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 42(3), 874–880.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kecklund, G., & Åkerstedt, T. (1993). Sleepiness in long distance truck driving: An ambulatory EEG study of night driving. Ergonomics, 36(9), 1007–1017.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, D., & Gross, J. J. (1999). Functional accounts of emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 13(5), 467–480.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, D., Haidt, J., & Shiota, M. N. (2006).Social functionalism and the evolution of emotions. In M. Schaller, J. A. Simpson, & D. T. Kenrick (eds.), Evolution and Social Psychology (pp. 115–142). New York: Psychology Press.

  • Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science, 330, 932.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klonowicz, T. (1987). Reactivity and the control of arousal. In J. Strelau & H. J. Eysenck (Eds.), Personality dimensions and arousal (pp. 197–216). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroes, S. (2005). Detecting boredom in meetings (pp. 1–5). Enschede: University of Twente.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lal, S. K., & Craig, A. (2002). Driver fatigue: Electroencephalography and psychological assessment. Psychophysiology, 39(3), 313–321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, R. W., & Richards, M. H. (1991). Boredom in the middle school years: Blaming schools versus blaming students. American Journal of Education, 99, 418–443.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazzaro, I., Gordon, E., Whitmont, S., Plahn, M., Li, W., Clarke, S., et al. (1998). Quantified EEG activity in adolescent attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clinical EEG (electroencephalography), 29(1), 37–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leary, M. R., Rogers, P. A., Canfield, R. W., & Coe, C. (1986). Boredom in interpersonal encounters: Antecedents and social implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 968–975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, T. W. (1986). Toward the development and validation of a measure of job measurement. Manhattan College Journal of Business, 15, 22–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levenson, R. W. (1994). Human emotions: A functional view. In P. Ekman & R. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 123–126). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewinsky, H. (1943). Boredom. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 13, 147–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lhommet, M., & Marsella, S. C. (2015). Expressing emotion through posture and gesture. In R. Calvo, S. D’Mello, J. Gratch, & A. Kappas (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of affective computing (pp. 273–285). Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lohrmann, K. (2008). Langeweile im Unterricht (Boredom in class). Miinster: Waxmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • London, H., & Monello, L. (1974). Cognitive manipulation of boredom. In H. London & R. Nisbett (Eds.), Thought and feeling (pp. 44–59). Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • London, H., Schubert, D. S., & Washburn, D. (1972). Increase of autonomic arousal by boredom. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 80, 29–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, U., Melin, B., Evans, G. W., & Holmberg, L. (1993). Physiological deactivation after two contrasting tasks at a video display terminal: Learning vs. repetitive data entry. Ergonomics, 36, 601–611.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malkovsky, E., Merrifield, C., Goldberg, Y., & Danckert, J. (2012). Exploring the relationship between boredom and sustained attention. Experimental Brain Research, 221(1), 59–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann, S., & Cadman, R. (2014). Does being bored make us more creative?.Creativity. Research Journal, 26(2), 165–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann, S., & Robinson, A. (2009). Boredom in the lecture theatre: An investigation into the contributors, moderators and outcomes of boredom amongst university students. British Educational Research Journal, 35(2), 243–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, M., Sadlo, G., & Stew, G. (2006). The phenomenon of boredom. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(3), 193–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matell, M. S., & Meck, W. H. (2000). Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behaviour. BioEssays, 22, 94–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matell, M. S., & Meck, W. H. (2004). Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: Coincidence-detection of oscillatory processes. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 21, 139–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, B., D'Mello, S., King, B., Chipman, P., Tapp, K., & Graesser, A. (2007, January). Facial features for affective state detection in learning environments. Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society, 29(29), 467–472.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meck, W. H. (1983). Selective adjustment of the speed of the clock and memory processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 171–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercer, K. B., & Eastwood, J. D. (2010). Is boredom associated with problem gambling behaviour? It depends on what you mean by ‘boredom’. International Gambling Studies, 10(1), 91–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merrifield, C., & Danckert, J. (2014). Characterizing the psychophysiological signature of boredom. Experimental Brain Research, 232(2), 481–491.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mikulas, W. L., & Vodanovich, S. J. (1993). The essence of boredom. Psychological Record, 43(1), 3–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, W. N. (1989). Mood: The frame of mind. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 247–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Musharbash, Y. (2007). Boredom, time, and modernity: An example from aboriginal Australia. American Anthropologist, 109(2), 307–317.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nederkoorn, C., Vancleef, L., Wilkenhöner, A., Claes, L., & Havermans, R. C. (2016). Self-inflicted pain out of boredom. Psychiatry Research, 237, 127–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nesselroade, J. R. (1988). Some implications of the trait–state distinction for the study of development over the life span: The case of personality. In P. B. Baltes, D. L. Featherman, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 8, pp. 163–189). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nett, U. E., Goetz, T., & Daniels, L. M. (2010). What to do when feeling bored?: Students' strategies for coping with boredom. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(6), 626–638.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neu, J. (1998). Boring from within: Endogenous versus reactive boredom. In W. F. Flack & J. D. Laird (Eds.), Emotions in psychopathology: Theory and research (pp. 158–170). London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ng, A. H., Liu, Y., Chen, J. Z., & Eastwood, J. D. (2015). Culture and state boredom: A comparison between European Canadians and Chinese. Personality and Individual Differences, 75, 13–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noulhiane, M., Mella, N., Samson, S., Ragot, R., & Pouthas, V. (2007). How emotional auditory stimuli modulate time perception. Emotion, 7(4), 697–704.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowlis, V., & Nowlis, H. H. (1956). The description and analysis of mood. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 65, 345–355.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Hanlon, J. F. (1981). Boredom: Practical consequences and a theory. Acta Psychologica, 49, 161–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oatley, K., & Jenkins, J. M. (1996). Understanding emotions. Cambridge: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). Towards a cognitive theory of emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 20–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Brien, W. (2014). Boredom. Analysis, 74, 236–244. doi:10.1093/analys/anu041.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ornstein, R. E. (1970). On the experience of time. Baltimore: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oswald, I. (1962). The EEG of sleep. In I. Oswald (Ed.), Sleeping and waking: Physiology and psychology (pp. 35–41). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parkinson, B., Totterdell, P., Briner, R. B., & Reynolds, S. (1996). Changing moods: The psychology of mood and mood regulation. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Passik, S. D., Inman, A., Kirsch, K., Theobald, D., & Dickerson, P. (2003). Initial validation of a scale to measure purposelessness, understimulation, and boredom in cancer patients: Toward a redefinition of depression in advanced disease. Palliative and Supportive Care, 1, 41–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pattyn, N., Neyt, X., Henderickx, D., & Soetens, E. (2008). Psychophysiological investigation of vigilance decrement: Boredom or cognitive fatigue? Physiology & Behavior, 93(1), 369–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peeters, G., & Czapinski, J. (1990). Positive-nega- tive asymmetry in evaluations: The distinction be- tween affective and informational negativity ef- fects. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Eu- ropean review of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 33–60). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Daniels, L. M., Stupnisky, R. H., & Perry, R. P. (2010). Boredom in achievement settings: Exploring control-value antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected emotion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(3), 531–549.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, R. E., & Hill, A. B. (1985). Cognitive and affective aspects of boredom. The British Psychological Society, 76, 221–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phipps-Nelson, J., Redman, J. R., & Rajaratnam, S. M. (2011). Temporal profile of prolonged, night-time driving performance: Breaks from driving temporarily reduce time-on-task fatigue but not sleepiness. Journal of Sleep Research, 20(3), 404–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pouthas, V., & Perbal, S. (2004). Time perception does not only depend on accurate clock mechanisms but also on unimpaired attention and memory processes. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 64, 367–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poynter, W. D. (1983). Duration judgment and the segmentation of experience. Memory & Cognition, 11, 77–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raccanello, D., & Bianchetti, C. (2014). Pictorial representations of achievement emotions: Preliminary data with children and adults. In T. Mascio, R. Gennari, P. Vitorini, R. Vicari, & F. de la Prieta (Eds.), Methodologies and Intelligent Systems for Technology Enhanced Learning (pp. 127–134). Springer International Publishing.

  • Ragheb, M. G., & Merydith, S. P. (2001). Development and validation of a unidimensional scale measuring free time boredom. Leisure Studies, 20, 41–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raposa, M. L. (1999). Boredom and the religious imagination. Charlottesville: University of Virginia Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reisenzein, R. (2000). Exploring the strength of association between the components of emotion syndromes: The case of surprise. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 1–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, W. P. (1975). Boredom at school. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 45(2), 141–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roseman, I. J. (1984). Cognitive determinants of emotion: A structural theory. In P. Shaver (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology, Emotions, relationships, and health (Vol. 5, pp. 11–36). Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, E. L. (1998). Levels of analysis and the organization of affect. Review of General Psychology, 2, 247–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, E., & Ekman, P. (1994). Coherence between expressive and experiential systems in emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 201–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roy, D. (1959). " Banana Time": Job Satisfaction and Informal Interaction. Human Organization, 18(4), 158–168.

  • Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(4), 296–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubia, K. (2011). “cool” inferior frontostriatal dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder versus “hot” ventromedial orbitofrontal-limbic dysfunction in conduct disorder: A review. Biological Psychiatry, 69(12), e69–e87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, B. (1996). The conquest of happiness. New York: W. W Norton & Company Inc..

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sangal, R. B., & Sangal, J. M. (2015). Use of EEG Beta-1 power and theta/Beta ratio over Broca’s area to confirm diagnosis of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 46(3), 177–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sansone, C., Weir, C., Harpster, L., & Morgan, C. (1992). Once a boring task always a boring task? Interest as a self-regulatory mechanism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(3), 370–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scerbo, M. W., Greenwald, C. Q., & Sawin, D. A. (1992). Vigilance: It’s boring, it’s difficult and I can’t do anything about it. Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 36th Annual Meeting, 36, 1508–1512.

  • Scherer, K. R. (1993). Studying the emotion-antecedent appraisal process: An expert system approach. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 325–355.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (2013). Nonlinguistic vocal indicators of emotion and psychopathology. In C. Izard (Ed.), Emotions in personality and psychopathology (pp. 495–529). New York and London: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R., & Ellgring, H. (2007). Multimodal expression of emotion: Affect programs or componential appraisal patterns? Emotion, 7(1), 158–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schier, M. A. (2000). Changes in EEG alpha power during simulated driving: A demonstration. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 37, 155–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schueller, S. M. (2014). Person-activity fit in positive psychological interventions. In A. C. Parks & S. M. Schueller (Eds.), The Wiley Blackwell handbook of positive psychological interventions (pp. 385-399). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

  • Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), The handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 527–561). New York: Guilford Press.

  • Schwarz, N. (2000). Emotion, cognition, and decision making. Cognition & Emotion, 14(4), 433–440.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schweizer, T. S. (2006). The psychology of novelty-seeking, creativity and innovation: Neurocognitive aspects within a work-psychological perspective. Creativity and Innovation Management, 15(2), 164–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seel, R. T., & Kreutzer, J. S. (2003). Depression assessment after traumatic brain injury: An empirically based classification method. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 84(11), 1621–1628.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silvia, P. J. (2008). Interest—The curious emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(1), 57–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skowronski, M. (2012). When the bored behave badly (or exceptionally). Personnel Review, 41, 143–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skowronski, J. J., & Carlston, D. E. (1989). Negativity and extremity biases in impression formation: A review of explanation. Psychological Re- view, 105, 131–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, R. P. (1981). Boredom: A review. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 23, 329–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. A., & Lazarus, R. S. (1993). Appraisal components, core relational themes, and the emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 233–269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. L., Wagaman, J., & Handley, I. M. (2009). Keeping it dull or making it fun: Task variation as a function of promotion versus prevention focus. Motivation and Emotion, 33(2), 150–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D. (1966a). The effects of anxiety on complex learning and academic achievement. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety and behavior (pp. 361–398). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D. (1966b). Theory and research on anxiety. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety and behavior (pp. 3–20). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D. (1972). Anxiety as an emotional state. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety: Current trends in theory and research (Vol. 1, pp. 23–49). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steinberger, F., Moeller, A., & Schroeter, R. (2016). The antecedents, experience, and coping strategies of driver boredom in young adult males. Journal of Safety Research, 59, 69–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Struk, A. A., Scholer, A. A., & Danckert, J. (2016). A self-regulatory approach to understanding boredom proneness. Cognition and Emotion, 30(8), 1388–1401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sundberg, N. D., Latkin, C. A., Farmer, R. F., & Saoud, J. (1991). Boredom in young adults gender and cultural comparisons. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 22(2), 209–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taatgen, N. A., van Rijn, H., & Anderson, J. R. (2007). An integrated theory of prospective time interval estimation: The role of cognition, attention and learning. Psychological Review, 114, 577–598.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabatabaie, A. F., Azadehfar, M. R., Mirian, N., Noroozian, M., Yoonessi, A., & Yoonessi, A. (2014). Neural correlates of boredom in music perception. Basic and Clinical Neuroscience, 5(4), 259–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thackray, R. I., Bailey, J. P., & Touchstone, R. M. (1977). Physiological, subjective and performance correlates of reported boredom and monotony while performing a simulated radar control task. FAA Office of Aviation Medicine Reports, 9, 75–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todman, M. (2003). Boredom and psychotic disorders: Cognitive and motivational issues. Psychiatry, 66, 146–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todman, M. (2013). The dimensions of state boredom: Frequency, duration, unpleasantness, consequences and causal attributions. Educational Research International, 1(1), 32–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2008). The evolutionary psychology of the emotions and their relationship to internal regulatory variables. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 103–129). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, M. (1963). Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implications for a model of the “internal clock”. Psychological Monographs, 77, 1–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, M., Faulkner, A., Naish, P. L. N., & Brogan, D. (1990). The internal clock: Evidence for a temporal oscillator underlying time perception with some estimates of its characteristic frequency. Perception, 19, 705–743.

    Google Scholar 

  • Troutwine, R., & O’Neal, E. C. (1981). Volition, performance of a boring task and time estimation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 52(3), 865–866.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tze, V. M. C., Daniels, L. M., Klassen, R. M., & Johnson, C.-H. L. (2013). Canadian and Chinese university students’ approaches to coping with academic boredom. Learning and Individual Differences, 23, 32–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Aart, J., Bartneck, C., Hu, J., Rauterberg, M., & Salem, B. (2010). How to behave as Alice in wonderland–about boredom and curiosity. Entertainment Computing, 1, 125–137. doi:10.1016/j.entcom.2010.09.003.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Hooff, M. L., & van Hooft, E. A. (2014). Boredom at work: Proximal and distal consequences of affective work-related boredom. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 19(3), 348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Tilburg, W. A., & Igou, E. R. (2011). On boredom and social identity: A pragmatic meaning-regulation approach. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 1679–1691.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Tilburg, W. A., & Igou, E. R. (2012). On boredom: Lack of challenge and meaning as distinct boredom experiences. Motivation and Emotion, 36(2), 181–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Tilburg, W. A., & Igou, E. R. (2016). Going to political extremes in response to boredom. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46(6), 687–699.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Tilburg, W. A., & Igou, E. R. (2017a). Boredom begs to differ: Differentiation from other negative emotions. Emotion, 17(2), 309–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Tilburg, W. A., & Igou, E. R. (2017b). Can boredom help? Increased prosocial intentions in response to boredom. Self and Identity, 16(1), 82–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Tilburg, W. A., Igou, E. R., & Sedikides, C. (2013). In search of meaningfulness: Nostalgia as an antidote to boredom. Emotion, 13(3), 450–461.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vodanovich, S. J. (2003). Psychometric measures of boredom: A review of the literature. The Journal of Psychology, 137(6), 569–595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vodanovich, S. J., & Kass, S. J. (1990). A factor analytic study of the boredom proneness scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55, 115–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vodanovich, S. J., & Watt, J. D. (2015). Self-report measures of boredom: An updated review of the literature. The Journal of Psychology, 150(2), 196–228. doi:10.1080/00223980.2015.1074531.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vodanovich, S. J., Watt, J. D., & Piotrowski, C. (1997). Boredom proneness in African-American college students: A factor analytic perspective. Education, 118(2), 229–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogel-Walcutt, J. J., Fiorella, L., Carper, T., & Schatz, S. (2012). The definition, assessment, and mitigation of state boredom within educational settings: A comprehensive review. Educational Psychology Review, 24(1), 89–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wackermann, J., & Ehm, W. (2006). The dual klepsydra model of internal time representation and time reproduction. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 239, 482–493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, E. L. (1980). Psychological complexity and preference. Monterey: Brooks/Cole.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, J. C., Vodanovich, S. J., & Restino, B. M. (2003). Predicting cognitive failures from boredom proneness and daytime sleepiness scores: An investigation within military and undergraduate samples. Personality and Individual Differences, 34(4), 635–644.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallbott, H. G. (1998). Bodily expression of emotion. European Journal of Psychology, 28, 879–896.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wangh, M. (1975). Boredom in psychoanalytic perspective. Sociological Research, 42, 538–550.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watt, J. D. (1991). Effect of boredom proneness on time perception. Psychological Reports, 69(1), 323–327.

  • Wearden, J., & Penton-Voak, I. (1995). Feeling the heat: Body temperature and the rate of subjective time, revisited. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. B, 48, 129–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, L., Xie, X., & Cleanthous, C. C. (1995). Purpose in life, boredom, and volunteerism in a group of retirees. Psychological Reports, 76(2), 482–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weissinger, E., Caldwell, L. L., & Bandalos, D. L. (1992). Relation between intrinsic motivation and boredom in leisure time. Leisure Sciences, 14, 317–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1336–1346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., Reinhard, D. A., Westgate, E. C., Gilbert, D. T., Ellerbeck, N., Hahn, C., Brown, C. L., & Shaked, A. (2014). Social psychology. Just think: the challenges of the disengaged mind. Science, 345, 75–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittmann, M. (2009). The inner experience of time. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 1955–1967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittmann, M., & van Wassenhove, V. (2009). The experience of time: Neural mechanisms and the interplay of emotion, cognition and embodiment. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 1809–1813.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yoshida, K., Sawamura, D., Inagaki, Y., Ogawa, K., Ikoma, K., & Sakai, S. (2014). Brain activity during the flow experience: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Neuroscience Letters, 573, 30–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (2004). Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: An executive-control perspective. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 64(3), 319–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (1997). Temporal cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6(1), 12–16.

  • Zhao, C., Zhao, M., Liu, J., & Zheng, C. (2012). Electroencephalogram and electrocardiograph assessment of mental fatigue in a driving simulator. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 45, 83–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M. (1976). General and situation-specific traits and states: New approaches to assessment of anxiety and other constructs. In M. Zuckerman & C. D. Spielberger (Eds.), Emotion and anxiety: New concepts, methods, and applications (pp. 133–174). Hillsdale: Errbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimal level of arousal. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M. (1983). The distinction between trait and state scales is not arbitrary: Comment on Allen and Potkay's "on the arbitrary distinction between traits and states.". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1083–1086.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M. (1987). A critical look at three arousal constructs in personality theories: Optimal levels of arousal, strength of the nervous system, and sensitivities to signals of reward and punishment. In J. Strelau & H. J. Eysenck (Eds.), Personality dimensions and arousal (pp. 217–232). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andreas Elpidorou.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Elpidorou, A. The bored mind is a guiding mind: toward a regulatory theory of boredom. Phenom Cogn Sci 17, 455–484 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-017-9515-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-017-9515-1

Keywords

Navigation