Skip to main content
Log in

On boredom: Lack of challenge and meaning as distinct boredom experiences

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Boredom is a common experience that affects people on multiple levels, including their thoughts, feelings, motivations, and actions. Not much research, however, has examined what makes the experience of boredom distinct from other affective experiences. Based on earlier research on boredom and our meaning-regulation framework, we conducted a series of four studies that demonstrate the distinct experiential content of boredom. More than other negative affective experiences (sadness, anger, and frustration), boredom makes people feel unchallenged while they think that the situation and their actions are meaningless (Study 1). The distinct experiential content of boredom is associated with boredom proneness (Study 2) and with state boredom experiences (Study 3). In addition, the distinct experiential content of boredom is affected by contextual features (Study 4). This series of studies provides a systematic understanding of what people feel, think, and want to do when bored, distinctive from other negative experiences.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Following Roseman et al. (1994), the term ‘emotivational goal’ refers to motivational components that are specifically related to emotions.

  2. For the items that did not work in their original research (e.g., actions of frustration) we developed items in order to have an equal amount of items for each emotion. Details can be provided on request.

  3. Across all studies, no main or interaction effects of gender and age were observed besides a single very small significant association between age and the boredom item “want to do something meaningful” of Study 1. Specifically, a 2-way ANCOVA with this item as dependent variable and the emotion condition and gender as independent variables revealed that participants’ endorsement of the item increased with age. F(1, 94) = 3.86, p = .05, η2 = .04 (B = .08, S e = .04). This association, however, did not interfere with the effect of interest.

  4. Even though the experiential content of sadness, anger, and frustration were not of primary interest for the current investigation, the original findings by Roseman et al. (1994) were largely replicated. Details can be provided on request. Interestingly, the sadness item ‘…feel very tired’ was found to be significantly more endorsed for boredom compared to each of the other emotions (all ps < .05). Although our investigation focused primarily on challenge and meaning, this item may be considered for inclusion in future research.

  5. No adjustments to the α-level were made because we made explicit predictions for each of the comparisons. Specifically, we predicted that participants would give higher ratings for the ten boredom items in the boredom condition relative to participants in the sadness, anger, or frustration conditions. Similarly, Roseman et al. (1994), whose approach served as basis for our Study 1, explain why adjustments would not be adequate (p. 208). However, to rule out any concerns in this regard, we checked the data after making Bonferroni corrections to the α-level. First of all, we correcting for the total amount of 12 ANOVAs by adopting an α-level of .004. Importantly, the critical ANOVAs associated with the seven distinct items, the ANOVA on the entire boredom content, and also the ANOVA on the composite of the distinct boredom content remained significant (all ps ≤ .002). We also examined the specific comparisons after adopting an α-level of .008, correcting for the 6 specific comparisons after each of the ANOVA’s. Importantly, people’s scores in the boredom condition were still significantly higher compared to those in the other conditions for each of the seven distinct boredom items, the entire boredom content, and also the composite of boredom’s distinct content (all ps ≤ .007).

  6. It should be noted that the identified category frequencies were too small for testing the statistical reliability of these differences.

  7. The sample size was relatively small compared to the other studies. Please note, however, that the study contained only two conditions and that the effect sizes of the predicted significant main effects are considerable (.13 ≤ all η2s ≤ .19). Moreover, sample sizes such as those in Study 4 are quite common is social psychology experiments that are part of a series of studies (e.g., Twenge et al. 2007, p. 57).

  8. Correlation analyses indicated that state boredom was significantly correlated with anger, r = .47, p < .01, and frustration, r = .64, p < .001, not with sadness, r = .19, p = .27. Moreover, anger significantly correlated with sadness, r = .61, p < .001, and frustration, r = .76, and also sadness and frustration were significantly correlated, r = .47, p < .01. These correlations are consistent with the general notice that the four experiences share elements and these correlations further subscribe to the importance of identifying how these emotional states can be distinguished from each other, as done in our research. Importantly, our manipulation affected boredom in particular and did not significantly alter levels of sadness, anger, and frustration.

  9. The mediation analysis procedure by Preacher and Hayes (2008) does not include β-coefficients in the output. We have therefore estimated the Beta’s by performing the same mediation analyses after standardizing the involved variables.

References

  • Acee, T. W., Kim, H., Kim, H. J., Kim, J. I., Chu, H. R., Kim, M., et al. (2010). Academic boredom in under- and over-challenging situations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35, 17–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barbalet, J. M. (1999). Boredom and social meaning. British Journal of Sociology, 50, 631–646.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bargdill, R. W. (2000). The study of life boredom. Journal of Phenomenological Psychology, 31, 188–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blaszczynski, A., McConaghy, N., & Frankova, A. (1990). Boredom proneness in pathological gambling. Psychological Reports, 67, 35–42.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L., & Bar-Anan, Y. (2007). Affect-as-information. In R. Baumeister & K. Vohs (Eds.), Encyclopedia of social psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Beyond boredom and anxiety: Experiencing flow in work and play. San Fransisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahlen, E. R., Martin, R. C., Ragan, K., & Kuhlman, M. M. (2004). Boredom proneness in anger and aggression: Effects of impulsiveness and sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1615–1627.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eastwood, J. D., Cavaliere, C., Fahlman, S. A., & Eastwood, A. E. (2007). A desire for desires: Boredom and its relation to alexithymia. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 1035–1045.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fahlman, S. A., Mercer, K. B., Gaskovski, P., Eastwood, A. E., & Eastwood, J. D. (2009). Does a lack of meaning cause boredom? Results from psychometric, longitudinal, and experimental analyses. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28, 307–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farmer, R., & Sundberg, N. D. (1986). Boredom proneness—the development and correlates of a new scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 4–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, C. D. (1998). Effects of external and internal interruptions on boredom at work: Two studies. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 19, 503–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotion. American Psychologist, 43, 349–358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (2007). The laws of emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & Ter Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 212–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fromm, E. (1973). The anatomy of human destructiveness. New York: Holt McDougal.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, A., Wilkinson, R., McGrown, A., & Jovanoska, S. (1997). The psychometric properties of the boredom proneness scale: An examination of its validity. Psychological Studies, 42, 85–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J., Koole, S. L., & Pyszczynski, T. (2004). Handbook of experimental existential psychology. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, J. J., & Segal, D. R. (1985). Observations from the Sinai: The boredom factor. Armed Forces and Society, 37, 235–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis is the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76, 408–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heine, S. J., Proulx, T., & Vohs, K. D. (2006). The meaning maintenance model: On the coherence of social motivations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 88–110.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kanevsky, L., & Keighley, T. (2003). To produce or not to produce? Understanding boredom and the honor in underachievement. Roeper Review, 26, 20–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kass, S. J., Vadanovich, S. J., & Callender, A. (2001). State-trait boredom: Relationship to absenteeism, tenure, and job satisfaction. Journal of Business and Psychology, 16, 317–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klapp, O. E. (1986). Overload and boredom: Essays on the quality of life in the information society. New York: Greenwood Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. S., & Smith, C. A. (1988). Knowledge and appraisal in the cognition-emotion relationship. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 281–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leary, M. R., Rogers, P. A., Canfield, R. W., & Coe, C. (1986). Boredom in interpersonal encounters: Antecedents and social implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 958–975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nett, U. E., Goetz, T., & Hall, N. C. (2011). Coping with boredom in school: An experience sampling perspective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, 49–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Daniels, L. M., Stupnisky, R. H., & Perry, R. P. (2010). Boredom in achievement settings: Control-value antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected emotion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 531–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavioral Research Methods, 40, 879–891.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roseman, I. J., Wiest, C., & Swartz, T. S. (1994). Phenomenology, behaviors, and goal differentiate discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 206–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rupp, D. E., & Vodanovich, S. J. (1997). The role of boredom proneness in self-reported anger and aggression. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 12, 925–936.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sansone, C., Weir, C., Harpster, L., & Morgan, C. (1992). Once a boring task always a boring task? Interest as a self-regulatory strategy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 379–390.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (1997). The role of culture in emotion-antecedent appraisal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 902–922.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2007). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In E. T. Higgins & A. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology. A handbook of basic principles (2nd ed., pp. 385–407). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silvia, P. J. (2006). Exploring the psychology of interest. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 813–838.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. L., Wagaman, J., & Handley, I. M. (2009). Keeping it dull or making it fun: Task variation as a function of promotion versus prevention focus. Motivation and Emotion, 33, 150–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stickney, M. L., & Miltenberger, R. G. (1999). Evaluating direct and indirect measures for the functional assessment of binge eating. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 26, 195–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Twenge, J. M., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Ciarocco, N. J., & Bartels, J. M. (2007). Social exclusion decreases prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 56–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van de Ven, N., Zeelenberg, M., & Pieters, R. (2009). Leveling up and down: The experiences of benign and malicious envy. Emotion, 9, 419–429.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Tilburg, W. A. P., & Igou, E. R. (2011). Why bored George helps others; A pragmatic meaning-regulation hypothesis on boredom and prosocial behavior. Under Review.

  • Van Tilburg, W. A. P., & Igou, E. R. (in press). On boredom and social identity: A pragmatic meaning-regulation approach. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.

  • Vodanovich, S. J. (2003). Psychometric properties of boredom: A review of the literature. Journal of Psychology, 137, 569–595.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vodanovich, S. J., & Kass, S. J. (1990). A factor analytic study of the boredom proneness scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55, 115–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vodanovich, S. J., Verner, K. M., & Gilbride, T. V. (1991). Boredom proneness: Its relationship to positive and negative affect. Psychological Reports, 69, 1139–1146.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wallbott, H. G. (1998). Bodily expression of emotion. European Journal of Psychology, 28, 879–896.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was in part funded by a PhD scholarship grant from the Irish Research Council for the Social Sciences and Humanities (IRCHSS). We want to thank Marcel Zeelenberg for his encouraging remarks on our undertaking, the members of the Social Cognition and Decision Making Lab for their constructive comments on our research, and Annemieke J. M. van den Tol for her assistance in data collection. In addition, we would also like to thank Timothy D. Ritchie for his feedback on an earlier version of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wijnand A. P. van Tilburg.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

van Tilburg, W.A.P., Igou, E.R. On boredom: Lack of challenge and meaning as distinct boredom experiences. Motiv Emot 36, 181–194 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9234-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9234-9

Keywords

Navigation